Sunday 23 September 2012

Catalyst: The Movie Revolt

The world has generated to a point where a low budget movie has become a catalyst to revolutions the world over. The world has seen its share of revolts since its inception. We can recall prominent uprisings like those of 1848 when it was opined that France sneezed and Europe caught a cold. The most recent was the Arab uprisings that wrecked the middle East and is still ravaging Syria till date. Then we thought that the world was tired of wars but that seems not to be the case evident in the enthusiasm with which people rushed back to the streets to react against the infamous movie.
It is factual that abusing anyone’s religious belief is in poor taste and really tacky. The irony is that we do that all the times since we always consider that ours is the best of faiths. Moslems in over fifteen countries have gone to the streets to show that rage because an individual made a low budget movie and abused the prophet Mohamed. Agreed, if you are outraged you let it known. The question is against whom? The United States is taking the brunt for the ‘sins’ of the movie maker. The US government did not sponsor the movie, but it doesn’t matter considering the fact that these individuals are not really revolting against the supposedly insulting movie. They are airing their frustration, hatred, envy and idleness to the world at large.
 Let’s track the origin of these revolts and make conclusions, the first was in Egypt, a country that just emerged from the ashes of a revolution, a country where in the Moslem brotherhood in power, a country where the military still holds all the real power. Let’s take Libya, Benghazi for another reference. We will all agree that Libya is still drunk with the heady feeling of freedom, they were under one tyrant for over forty years. Then they shoved him off, Benghazi was celebrated as a fount of freedom but now it is the home of idleness. The same revolutionaries who took arms against Khadafy are still armed and jobless. This movie thus gave an opportunity for them to be occupied. It is completely shameful that they should go after innocent people who are only doing their jobs. By killing the US ambassador, these demented fanatics claimed to have avenged the insult that was brought upon the prophet by the movie. It is totally stupid since the US government and especially the ambassador did not make the movie. In fact they were not aware of its existence until they watched the burning of US flags on the BBC. Now other countries are striking in sympathy, talk about Kenya, why will they be protesting for? How many good Moslems are in that country again? Then there is Sudan, they are not done with civil war yet, do they really have the energy to protest about some cheesy movie? We all know Moslems in Nigeria are reactionary and find the least opportunity to cause trouble. Then what about Tunisia? their government is condoning such reaction because they have been unable to reorganize the country after the recent uprising.
Then someone in jest questioned why Moslems in Cameroon have not joined to remonstrate in sympathy as well. I say that is a laugh because Moslems in Cameroon know it is totally uncalled for to try such a thing. Our government allows freedom of speech and expression as long as you don’t disturb the peace. If anyone should attempt to go to the streets for such a flimsy reason as sympathy or any other reason, the Rapid Intervention Brigade will be unleashed from its cage and be permitted to have a hay day. Those guys are like dementors, they are only happy when they are causing someone unhappiness. And those gentlemen really know how to make someone sing, George Bush could learn a few tricks from them when he was questioning terrorists in Guantanamo Bay. For the world to standstill for such a stupid reason, I am tempted to agree with Pentecostals that the end time is near. It is funny that the same Moslems who have been persecuting Coptic Christians, burning their churches and killing them should be so surprise that one of such Christians should express his anguish and disgust albeit in an underrated manner.



Creativity, the Arts, and Madness


By Maureen Neihart, Psy.D.

Abstract
A brief, historical review of the alleged association between creativity and madness is followed by highlights from recent research in psychiatry and clinical  psychology that address this relationship.
The precise nature of  this link is explored from the perspectives of several disciplines, and implications for the creative process in  gifted education are discussed.
Creativity is defined as the production of something both new and valued.  Madness is defined as self destructive deviant behavior.

Men have called me mad; but the question is not yet settled, whether madness is or is not the loftiest intelligence-whether much that is glorious-whether all that is profound-does not spring from disease of thought-from moods of mind exalted at the expense of the general intellect. 

They who dream by day are cognizant of many things which escape those who dream only by night.  In their grey vision they obtain glimpses of eternity.... They penetrate, however rudderless or compassless, into the vast ocean of the "light affable."

(Edgar Allan Poe, cited in Galloway, 1986, p. 243 )

.
The belief that madness is  linked with creative thinking has been held since ancient times. It is a widely popular notion. "Deviant behavior, whether in the form of eccentricity or worse, is not only associated with persons of genius or high-level creativity, but it is frequently expected of them." (Rothenberg, 1990, p. 149).
Since the time of the Greek philosophers, those who wrote about the creative process emphasized that creativity involves a regression to more primitive mental processes, that to be creative requires a willingness to cross and recross  the lines between rational and irrational thought.

What is the evidence that there is a link between creativity and madness? What account can be given for this link, biologically and psychologically? And what does this association suggest for related research and our understanding of creative people?

The aim of this article is to describe what creativity and madness have in common and to discuss implications for creative thinking  in gifted education. The article begins with a brief, historical overview of the topic, followed by some highlights of studies on creativity and mental illness. Explanations for the possible link between creativity and madness are then addressed.
Creativity is defined as the production of something that is both new and valued and madness is defined as a self destructive deviation in behavior. The article concludes with a discussion of implications for the creative process in gifted education and questions for further research.

Historical Overview

The notion that  inspiration requires  regression and dipping into irrationality in order to access unconscious symbols and thought has been popular across disciplines for hundreds of years. Plato said that creativity is a "divine madness...a gift from the gods". 

Seneca recorded Aristotle as having said, "No great genius was without a mixture of insanity" (Langsdorf, 1900, pp. 90-91).
One of Shakespeare's characters says, "The lunatic, the lover and the poet are of imagination all compact," and Marcel Proust  said, "Everything great in the world is created by neurotics. They have composed our masterpieces, but we don't consider what they have cost their creators in sleepless nights, and worst of all, fear of death."      

More recently, at the end of the last century, physicians were very interested in the physical causes of mental illness as well as  in the genetic causes of genius.

The physician, Lombroso (1889), wrote about the connections he believed to exist between genius and madness.  Acceptance of  his ideas persisted well into the 20th century until Lewis Terman's (1925) data suggested that people of high ability exhibited less incidence of mental illness and adjustment problems than average.
But at the same time that Terman was beginning to publish the first round of his results,  Freud was formulating his psychoanalytic concepts in Vienna. Freud analyzed literary works and the lives of eminent creative people because, "He believed that great works of art and literature contained universal psychological truths and that the study of artists' and writers' lives would reveal basic psychological truths in persons of heightened sensibility and talent." (Rothenberg, 1990, p. 80).
Since the time of Freud's analyses, other  psychoanalysts and psychologists have continued to conduct scores of pathographies, diagnostic analyses of the works or lives of eminent creative people in an effort to improve our understanding of the relationship between creativity and madness (Jamison, 1993; Panter, et. al, 1995).

In this century the clinical  literature, particularly  the psychoanalytic writing, is full of theories about the relationship between creativity and emotional illness (Feldman, 1989; Greenacre, 1957; Jamison, 1993; Lowenfeld, 1941; Niederland, 1976; Panter, Panter, Virshup and Virshup, 1995; Pickford, 1981; Richards, 1981; Rothenberg, 1990). 

A long-held view in psychiatry is that artistic endeavors heal the artist, whose work is then healing to others. It is important to note that the studies tend to focus on a subpopulation of artists in particular: writers, poets, and visual artists.

There are numerous examples of artists who used their work to save their minds. For example, Anne Sexton, who was institutionalized for her psychosis wrote, "Poetry led me by the hand out of madness" (cited in Jamison, 1993, p. 122) and  Jackson Polluck's large canvas drippings have been viewed by several investigators  as an attempt to organize his chaotic inner life (Feldman, 1989; Virshup, 1995; Wyshup, 1970). 

A basic premise of the expressive therapies (e.g. art, music, and dance therapy, etc.) is that writing, composing, or drawing, etc., is a means to self-understanding, emotional stability and resolution of conflict. Creativity provides a way to structure or reframe pain. This,  perhaps, is what much good comedy is about.

Findings From Studies on Creativity and Mental Illness

In the last two decades there have been numerous systematic investigations into the alleged relationship between creativity and madness. Albert Rothenberg, Kay Jamison,  and Nancy Andreasen are a sample of investigators who have explored this topic.

What do creativity and madness have in common? Observations from psychiatric studies suggest that there are three characteristics common to both high creative production and madness.  These are disturbance of mood, certain types of thinking processes, and tolerance for irrationality.

Disturbance of mood appears to be present in a high percentage of talented visual artists (Andreasen, 1988; Jamison,1989; 1993; Richards, 1981). Mental disorders in which the primary feature is a mood disturbance include major depression, dysthymia and bipolar disorder (also popularly  known as manic-depressive illness).

There seems to be a greatly increased rate of depression, manic-depressive illness, and suicide in eminent creative people, writers and artists especially.
The incidence of mental illness among creative artists is higher than in the population at large. Some studies link creativity with bipolar disorders specifically (Andreasen, 1988; Jamison, 1989; Richards; 1989), and within the field of academic psychiatry, there has recently been serious acceptance of the association between creativity and the mood disturbance, hypomania (Jamison, 1993).
Table1 lists a sample of eminent persons who are believed to have had a mood disorder. Many of them committed suicide.

It is well recognized that moods do have an impact on personality. Bipolar disorder is a recurrent mood disturbance characterized by cyclical, extreme mood swings that include manic states. Mania is a distinct period (at least a week) during which the individual demonstrates a euphoric high or irritable mood.

"The expansive quality of the mood is characterized by unceasing and indiscriminate enthusiasm for interpersonal, sexual, or occupational interactions" (APA, 1994, p. 328). Grandiosity or uncritical self confidence is often observed. During a manic state, thoughts race, sometimes faster than can be articulated.
There is a great increase in goal-directed activity. Manic individuals may write volumes, paint numerous canvases, or engage in multiple activities simultaneously. The level of activity is so high that it results in impairment of functioning,  or hospitalization may be necessary to protect the individual.

Jamison's (1993) work suggests that periods of creative productivity are preceded by an elevated mood. It is as if certain types of moods open up thought, allowing for greater creativity. She (1993) states that depressions may have an important cognitive influence on the creative process.

Depression may slow the pace, put thoughts and feelings into perspective; and eliminate excess or irrelevant ideas, increasing focus and allowing structuring of new ideas. In other words, it may be that the cognitive processes associated with certain moods are the link between creativity and madness.

Perhaps the most interesting finding from clinical studies is that there are similarities in the thought processes of manic,  psychotic, and highly creative people (Prentky, 1980; Rothenberg, 1990; Rothenberg & Burkhardt, 1984).

Psychotic thinking rarely turns into creative production without some abatement of the psychosis, but there is evidence that creative processes sometimes turn into psychotic ones. Albert Rothenberg is clinical professor of psychiatry at Harvard and has served for the past twenty-five years as principal investigator of the Studies in the Creative Process.
One focus of his research has been the relationship of creativity to psychosis. "I was at one time extraordinarily puzzled and piqued about the fact that so many outstanding persons also suffered from some form of psychosis (1990, p. 6). 

The major findings to come out of my research are that there are particular and specific thought processes used by creative people during the process of creation; this applies to the entire spectrum of disciplines, areas and media.

These special thought processes are the features that distinguish creative people from the rest of us. Although very complicated in structure and in psychological function, there is little doubt that these particular processes are crucial to outstanding creative attainment (1990, p. 11).

Specifically, Rothenberg's research concludes that translogical types of thinking characterize both psychotics and highly creatives. Translogical thinking, he explains, is a type of conceptualizing in which the thinking processes transcend the common modes of ordinary logical thinking.

It involves what Rothenberg calls janusian and homospatial processes. Janusian thinking is a conscious process of combining  paradoxical or antagonistic objects into a single entity. Homospatial process is the essence of good metaphor. It means to superimpose or bring together multiple, discrete objects. 
Rothenberg states that janusian thinking tends to occur in the beginning stages of creative work when ideas are generated, and homospatial thinking characterizes the development of the creative ideas. He acknowledges that there are similarities between the primary process thinking of psychotics and translogical thinking, and that there are some subtle  distinctions. 
"There is thus a thin but definite borderline between the most advanced and healthy type of thinking - creative thinking - and the most impoverished and pathological types of thinking - psychotic processes" (p. 12).

Other researchers have noted cognitive similarities. Drs. Andreasen, Stevens, and  Powers (1975) investigated conceptual overinclusiveness (i.e. the tendency to combine things into categories that blur conceptual boundaries) in a sample of writers, manic depressives and schizophrenics.

They found that the conceptual styles of only the first two groups were similar, with a difference being that the writers had more control over their thought processes than did the manic-depressives.

Kay Jamison's research (1989; 1993) also supports the idea that there is a cognitive link between creativity and madness. She notes that many of the cognitive changes that characterize mania and hypomania are also typical of creativity: restlesness, grandiosity, irritability, intensified sensory systems, quickening of thought processes, and intense feeling.

"Two aspects of thinking in particular are pronounced in both creative and hypomanic thought: fluency, rapidity, and flexibility of thought on the one hand, and the ability to combine ideas or categories of thought in order to form new and original connections on the other" (1993, p. 105).
It appears that the potential for creativity is enhanced by the cognitive changes that occur within some mental states. We don't as yet understand the chemical and anatomical pathways responsible for the cognitive changes that take place during creative and manic states.

Finally, insights concerning the relationship between creativity and madness come also from artists themselves. Their reflections and observations about themselves and their work suggest that they have a very high tolerance for irrationality or deviance. In life, creation and destruction are closely related.

Many artists report that their motivation for engaging in their creative endeavors is to work through, release, or better understand their own destructive urges.

The life and suicides of Sylvia Plath and Jackson Polluck exemplify how thin the line can be between destruction and creation.  Rothenberg (1990) hypothesizes that this line is crossed, from creativity to madness, when the creative expression is used primarily to control hostility rather than to create.
"Just as a need to control interferes with turning destructiveness into creation in art, so it interferes with turning self-destructive feelings into a process of self-creation in life" (p. 73).

Additionally, many artists personally attest to one of the most widely accepted associations between creativity and madness - the connection between what is learned from personal suffering to round out meaning and depth in the creative work. The poet, John Berryman for example, described the role of pain in his work:
I do strongly feel that among the greatest pieces of luck for high achievement is ordeal. Certain great artists can make out without it..., but mostly you need ordeal...My idea is this: The artist is extremely lucky who is presented with the worst possible ordeal which will not actually kill him. At that point, he's in business. Beethoven's deafness, Goya's deafness, Milton's blindness, that kind of thing.

And I think that what happens in my poetic work in the future will probably largely depend not on my sitting calmly on my ass as I think, 'Hmm, hmm, a long poem again? Hmm', but on kinds of other things short of senile dementia.  At that point, I'm out, but short of that, I don't know,  I hope to be nearly crucified. (cited in Plimpton, 1976, p. 322)

Discussion

In summary, there is evidence of a link between creativity and madness, especially within the subpopulation of writers, poets, and visual artists. There is a higher incidence of creatively gifted people among certain mental disorders than in the general population (Andreasen 1988; Jamison, 1989; 1993; Richards, 1989).

There seems to be an increased rate of suicide in eminent creative people. Many of the cognitive processes that characterize creative writing also characterize certain mood disorders. The conceptual style of writers and manic-depressives has been found to be similar.
And the personal accounts of many creative writers and visual artists testify to their struggle with psychological problems. These findings suggest that the line between creativity and madness is a fine one, and probably permeable.

The common ground between creativity and mental illness appears to be  intrapsychic conflict. Noting a few exceptions (e.g.,  Peter Paul Rubens), most creative people produce less during calm times in their lives (Berman, 1995). Artists  themselves argue that they strive to keep contact with their primitive selves because it is from their  core self that they draw the energy and inspiration needed to do their best work. 

But many mental health professionals would propose that to wrestle often with the primitive self is like walking the edge between sanity and insanity.  What implications does the research  have for educational practices?

There do appear to be psychological risks associated with creative giftedness and with the pursuit of exceptional creative achievement. Teachers and counselors should be aware of the vulnerability that can be associated with creative talent.

They can help students and parents guard against a too ready acceptance of the popular notion that deviant or destructive behaviors are the sine qua non of outstanding creative achievement. Suffering, or mental breakdown, should not be accepted as a likely consequence for creative production. Neither research or history supports that view.
At the same time, it is probably important that those working with the creatively gifted be willing to tolerate a higher degree of irrationality or deviance since such behaviors are more common among these individuals.
Although we should not ignore the strong association of certain types of psychological problems with creative production, neither should we ignore the observation that for every disturbed creative individual noted there are many more healthy creative individuals.                  

Educators need to understand and accept that the creative process does  often arouse considerable anxiety, which may interfere with production. The teacher who can anticipate this possibility and who can make accomodations that support the student in reducing anxiety will promote the student's achievement.

It would also be helpful if school personnel were at least aware that the thought processes of high creatives and those of manics or psychotics are similar on the surface, but very different foundationally.
Teachers and parents might advocate for acceptance of translogical modes of thinking, but not encourage the widely  popular pairing of creative achievement with destructive deviant behaviors.       

The research also suggests that differentiated emotional support should be available to students who are in pursuit of superior creative achievement.  Educators should increase awareness among students and their parents of the psychological risks common to the pursuit of superior creative achievement and assist them in developing strategies to minimize or prevent harm (Jamison, 1993; 1995; Markova, 1994; Rothenberg, 1990).

Both by their nature and by their identification with eminent artists, creatively gifted individuals may put themselves at risk for serious emotional disturbance. Specific assistance in managing mood vacillations may be helpful.
Self-care strategies designed for the artistic temperament may be beneficial in minimizing the damage that can occur when the line between rationality and irrationality is crossed and recrossed.

There are several resources that describe strategies for self-care. For example, David Wexler's Program for Innovative Self-Management (PRISM) is described in his text, The Adolescent Self: Strategies for Self-management, Self-soothing, and Self-esteem in Adolescents (Wexler,1991).

In addition, he has written two workbooks of exercises designed to help adolescents who are having  problems with self-destructive behaviors, anxiety, mood swings, aggression, substance abuse, and eating disorders (Wexler,1991;1993).

Several resources exist that describe exercises that could be used by experienced teachers or school guidance personnel to promote emotional health and prevent more serious problems among creatively gifted youth (Davis, McKay, & Eshelman, 1982; Ilardo, 1992; McMahon, 1992).

There are also resources describing exercises that might be especially relevant to students who are aspiring writers, actors, dancers and musicians (Heckler, 1985; Markova, 1994; Progoff, 1975).
Those training and teaching creatively gifted students should have in their referral network mental health professionals who can distinguish superior creative thinking from crazy, psychotic thinking and who can identify serious mood disturbances.

The creative process is a mystery. We can know about pieces of it, but we are unlikely to unravel all of it. Many questions remain unanswered.


If there is a significant correlation between creative genius and mental disorders, how do we explain it? Do mood disorders lead to creativity? 

Is there something about wrestling with the primitive core or with our moods, that facilitates the creative process? Or is there a vulnerability that accompanies creative thought? 
How do we explain the exceptions - those who achieve greatness and lead healthy lives? Are people with certain types of difficulties (e.g., mood disorders, substance abuse) more attracted to the creative fields than are people without such difficulties?
Is there something about the creative process itself that over time, contributes to disintegration? Or are the struggles for health the result of the cumulative effects of repeated interactions with others who lack understanding or tolerance?
Table 1

Eminent Creative People with Probable Mood Disorders

John Berryman                                    Honore De Balzac
Hans Christian Andersen                       Robert Burns
Samuel Clemens                                  Lord Byron
Charles Dickens                                   Samuel Taylor Coleridge          
Isak Dinesen                                       Emily Dickinson
Ralph Waldo Emerson                          T.S. Eliot
William Faulkner                                  Victor Hugo
F. Scott Fitzgerald                                John Keats
Ernest Hemingway                                Edna St. Vincent Millay
Henry James                                        Sylvia Plath
Eugene O'Neill                                      Edgar Allan Poe
Leo Tolstoy                                          Anne Sexton
Tennessee Williams                              Ezra Pound
Virginia Woolf                                       Alfred Lord Tennyson
Emile Zola                                            Dylan Thomas
Walt Whitman                                      Michelangelo
Irving Berlin                                          Jackson Pollock
Noel Coward                                        Vincent Van Gogh
Stephen Foster                                    Edvard Munch
Cole Porter                                          Mark Rothko
Paul Gauguin                                       Georgia O'Keeffe

Adapted from Jamison, 1993; Panter et al., 1995; and Rothenberg, 1990.

References

American Psychiatric Association, (1994).  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.  Washington D.C.:  author.

Andreasen, N. (1988). Bipolar affective disorder and creativity: implications and clinical management. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29(3) 207-217.

Berman, L. E. A. (1995).  An artist destroys his work: Comments on creativity and destructiveness.   In B. Panter  et. al (Eds.)  Creativity and madness.  Burbank, CA:  AIMED Press, pp.  59-80.

Davis, M., McKay, M., & Eshelman, E.R. (1982). The relaxation and stress reduction workbook. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.

Feldman, T. (1989).  Creativity and narcissism: a self-psychology examination of the life and work of Jackson Polluck.  The arts in psychotherapy. New York:  Pergamon Press.   

Galloway, D. (Ed.) (1986).  The fall of the house of usher and other writings.  London: Penguin.

Greenacre, P. (1957).  The childhood of the artist: libidinal phase development and giftedness.  Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 12, 42-72.

Heckler, R.S. (1985). The anatomy of change. Boston: Shambhala.

Ilardo, J. (1992). Risk-taking for personal growth: A step-by-step workbook. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications. 

Jamison, K.R. (1995).  An unquiet mind.  New York:  Alfred A. Knopf

Jamison, K.R. (1993).  Touched with fire:  Manic depressive illness and the artistic temperament. New York:  Free Press.

Jamison, K. (1989). Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British writers and artists.  Psychiatry, 52(2), 125-134.

Langsdorf, W.B.(1900). Tranquility of mind. New York: Putnam's Sons.

Lombroso, C. (1989).  L'Homme de genie. Paris:  Alcon.

Lowenfeld, H. (1941).  Psychic trauma and productive experience in the artist.  Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 10, 116-130.

Markova, D. (1994). No enemies within: A creative process for discovering what's right about what's wrong. Berkeley, CA: Conari Press.

McMahon, S. (1992). The portable therapist. New York: Dell Publishing.

Niederland, W. (1976).  Psychoanalytic approaches to artistic creativity. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 45, 185-212.

Panter, B., Panter, M., Virshup, E. and Virshup, B. (1995).  Creativity and madness: Psychological studies of art and artists. Burbank, CA:  American Institute of Medical Education.

Pickford, R.W. (1981). Art and Psychopathology.  In D.O'Hare (Ed.)  Psychology and the arts.  Sussex, New Jersey:  Harvester Press.

Plimpton, G. (Ed.) (1976).  Writers at work:  The Paris review interviews.  New York:  Viking Press.

Powers, P.S. , Stevens, C., & Andreasen, N.J. (1975).  The ontogenesis of intelligence: Evaluating the Piaget theory.  Comprehensive Psychiatry, 16(2), 149-154.

Prentky, R.A. (1980).  Creativity and psychopathology:  A neurocognitive perspective.  New York:  Praeger.

Progoff, I. (1975). At a journal workshop. New York: Dialogue House Library.
Richards, R. (1989). Compelling evidence for increased rates of affective disorder among eminent creative persons. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30(3), 272-273.

Richards, R. (1981). Relationships between creativity and psychopathology: An evaluation and interpretations of the evidence. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 103, 261-326.


Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity & madness: New findings and old stereotypes. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

Rothenberg, A. & Burkhardt, P.E. (1984). Difference in response time of creative persons and patients with depressive and schizophrenic disorders. Psychological Reports, 54, 711-717.


Sacks, O. (1995). An anthropologist on mars. New York: Vintage Books

Terman, L.M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius: Vol. 1. Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Stanford University, at Stanford, Calif.; coeducational; chartered 1885, opened 1891 as Leland Stanford Junior Univ. (still the legal name). The original campus was designed by Frederick Law Olmsted. David Starr Jordan was its first president.  Press.


Virshup, (1995). Jackson Pollock: Art vs. Alcohol. In B. Panter et. al (Eds.) Creativity and madness. Burbank, CA: AIMED Press, pp. 17-40.


Wexler, D. (1991a). The adolescent self. Strategies for self-management, self-soothing, and self-esteem in adolescents. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.


Wexler, D. (1991b). The PRISM workbook: A program for innovative self-management. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.


Wexler, D. (1993). The advanced PRISM workbook: Program for innovative self-management. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.


Wyshup, C. (1970). Jackson Pollock/psychoanalytic drawings. New York: Horizon

 




Saturday 22 September 2012

Nforche Gerald is an emerging Cameroon writer. He is a gifted cartoonist and poet who has published two poetry collections:

-Hera Me Sing 
-Phantoms of Somnolence

his short stories and poems have appeared in three editions of the Ngoh-Kuoh Review. he loves the gothic and writes thrillers and gothic.
He live sin Yaounde Cameroon where he reads English in the university of Yaounde I.

About Nforche Gerald